Method Article
* These authors contributed equally
We present development of a gaze-contingent display framework designed for perceptual and oculomotor research simulating central vision loss. This framework is particularly adaptable for studying compensatory behavioral and oculomotor strategies in individuals experiencing both simulated and pathological central vision loss.
Macular degeneration (MD) is one of the leading causes of vision impairment in the Western world. Patients with MD tend to develop spontaneous eye movement strategies to compensate for their vision loss, including adopting a preferred retinal locus, or PRL, a spared peripheral region that they use more frequently to replace the damaged fovea. However, not all patients are successful in developing a PRL, and even when they do, it might take them months. Currently, no gold standard rehabilitative therapy exists, and MD research is further hindered by issues of recruitment, compliance, and comorbidity. To help address these issues, a growing body of research has used eye tracking-guided, gaze-contingent displays in a simulated central vision loss paradigm in individuals with intact vision. While simulated vision loss is qualitatively different than pathological central vision loss, our framework provides for a highly controlled model through which to study compensatory eye movements and test possible rehabilitation interventions in low vision. By developing a comprehensive framework, rather than relying on isolated and disconnected tasks, we create a cohesive environment where we can test larger-scale hypotheses, allowing us to examine interactions between tasks, assess training effects across multiple measures, and establish a consistent methodology for future research. Furthermore, participants in simulated central vision loss studies show similarities in their oculomotor compensatory behaviors compared to patients with MD. Here, we present a framework for conducting gaze-contingent studies related to simulated central vision loss. We emphasize the utilization of the framework to test behavioral and oculomotor performance of healthy individuals on a wide range of perceptual tasks encompassing different levels of visual processing. We also discuss how this framework can be adapted for training MD patients.
Macular degeneration (MD) is the main cause of vision impairment globally, and it is projected to affect 248 million people worldwide by 20401. Late-stage MD is characterized by damage to the photoreceptors in the center of the visual field (fovea). Loss of central vision has severe effects on daily tasks that rely on central vision, such as navigation2, reading3, and recognizing faces4. Consequences of MD greatly impact the quality of life of these individuals5 and lead to negative psychological consequences6. Patients with MD, deprived of their central vision, may spontaneously develop compensatory oculomotor strategies involving the use of a peripheral retinal region to replace the fovea (Figure 1). This region, referred to as the preferred retinal locus (PRL)7, is often adopted by patients in tasks involving fixation, reading, and face recognition. There is evidence of the PRL, in patients with MD, taking over oculomotor referencing duties of the fovea8,9. Further, changes in attention and cognitive control are observed in patients with central vision loss, suggesting a relationship between vision loss and cognitive functions10.
Figure 1. Illustration of the perceptual experience of individuals with healthy vision and macular degeneration patients with foveal scotoma. Foveal scotoma leads to central vision loss in patients with macular degeneration. Some individuals can partially compensate for the loss of visual input to the fovea by using a peripheral retinal location, defined as preferred retinal locus (PRL). In patients that developed a PRL, this is often used for eccentric fixation and during daily tasks. Retinal location, shape and size of the PRL can vary from person to person. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
While no gold standard intervention exists to recover vision loss or to compensate for loss of central vision, experimental approaches from optometry, occupational therapy and vision science are being tested to improve compensation through peripheral vision11,12. Oculomotor approaches focus on teaching patients to improve eye movement control and coordination, including teaching them to use a more adequate PRL11,12,13,14,15 while perceptual interventions focus on improving the general peripheral visual abilities or vision within the PRL, partially overcoming the limitation of peripheral vision16,17,18,19,20. Recent studies have used an eye-tracking based gaze-contingent display as a paradigm for the study of eye movements in central vision loss21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29. This approach, which utilizes a simulated scotoma (i.e., an occluder to obstruct the central region of the visual field) in healthy individuals (Figure 1), mitigates issues of recruitment and compliance, while providing high control on several parameters, such as the size and shape of the scotoma, thereby offering a promising alternative to the direct involvement of patients with MD. While there exists several differences between central vision loss and simulated scotoma30,31, some of the oculomotor behavior observed in the former, such as the development of a PRL, can be seen in the latter27,30,32, suggesting that some aspects of compensatory oculomotor strategies can be elicited by this gaze-contingent paradigm. Importantly, simulated central vision loss provides a broad framework for studying plasticity in both the healthy visual system and following central vision loss.
Here, we present the design, development, and use of a gaze-contingent framework that can be used to test perceptual, oculomotor, and attentional performances in healthy individuals and, with some modifications, in MD patients (Figure 2). We also detail the technical and psychophysical considerations that accompany gaze-contingent, peripheral training. A key technical challenge involves creating the perception of a smooth, short latency movement of the scotoma33. This short latency is obtained by selecting appropriate display devices, eye trackers, and operating systems34,35,36. Previous work has documented how each piece of hardware adds latency37 and strategies to reduce overall latency, accommodate blinks, and slow eye movements33. A novel aspect of our paradigm is the diverse set of training and assessment tasks within a single framework for perceptual research in both healthy and patient populations. The framework characterizes multiple levels of visual processing affected by central vision loss, specifically low-level vision, higher-level vision, attention, oculomotor control, and cognitive control. Preliminary tests conducted using a modified version of this approach showed evidence of improvement in visual acuity in both healthy controls and the patient population32.
Figure 2. Multidimensional approach to the study of plasticity in the visual system, and vision rehabilitation in Macular Degeneration. Illustration of interconnected dimensions such as visual perception, oculomotor, and cognitive control that contribute to visual processing and are affected in central vision loss. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
All participants were healthy individuals with visual acuity of 20/40 or above and no known vision issues. Both the representative participants are females, and their ages are 27 and 24. All participants provided informed consent, and the study received approval from the Institutional Review Board (IRB) at the University of Alabama at Birmingham.
1. Identifying an ideal system for simulated central vision loss research
Figure 3: Latency comparison across different combinations of monitors, eye-tracking devices, and operating systems. Bars represent the ± 1 standard deviation across the 20 repetitions per combination. Measures were taken with a Mac operating system phone in slow motion mode, reaching a refresh rate of 240Hz. TP/CRS/Win is statistically different from E1000/CRT/Mac (t(38)=9.53, p<0.001), E1000/CRS/Mac (t(38)=16.24, p<0.001) and E1000/CRS/Win (t(38)=3.94, p<0.001). Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
2. Participant familiarization with simulated central vision loss through gaze-contingent display
NOTE: A fundamental component in simulating central vision loss is to familiarize participants with the gaze-contingent display. Without proper familiarization, measures of abilities can be conflated by the participants' effort to navigate the gaze-contingent display. Several key steps in the protocol ensure sufficient familiarization with the gaze-contingent display to be able to measure visual performance reliably.
3. Development of effective instructions
NOTE: Instructions play a crucial role in guiding participants on how to respond to stimuli and manage their simulated scotoma during different tasks. Appropriate instructions must be thorough and clear to avoid any confusion. Instructions should be reiterated as needed to ensure understanding.
4. Assessment tasks' design and implementation
NOTE: Tasks designed within this framework are broadly divided into two main categories: (1) Free eye movement tasks and (2) Fixation-constrained tasks. In the free-eye movement tasks, let participants make eye movements across the screen to identify targets appearing at random locations on the screen (or to read text), whereas, in fixation-constrained tasks, ask participants to maintain fixation within a central white box throughout the task and use their peripheral vision to make judgments. Figure 4 shows example tasks and descriptions for each category. More detailed information about the tasks can be found in38.
Figure 4: A visual representation of different assessment tasks designed using the framework. The tasks are broadly categorized into Free eye movement tasks, where the scotoma follows the eye movements of the participants to view targets freely (top panel), and Fixation-constrained tasks, where the scotoma needs to be placed within a central white box throughout the task (bottom panel). This figure has been modified from38. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 5: Fixation aids used to promote fixation stability in participants. (A) Fixation cross and fixation box were used for fixation stability tasks. (B) The fixation cross, fixation box, and black cross at the center were used in low-level vision tasks. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
In this section, we present illustrative data from both free eye movement and fixation-constrained tasks. The goal of this section is to illustrate data obtained using the framework and its ability to measure peripheral visual functions. The section is organized into four distinct categories, each highlighting critical elements necessary for accurate visual performance estimation under simulated central vision loss. These categories include performance on (1) low- and mid-level vision tasks, (2) attention measures in high-level vision tasks, (3) ecologically valid tasks, and (4) oculomotor metrics that capture adaptive eye movement strategies when central vision fixation is obstructed. All participants were healthy individuals with visual acuity of 20/40 or above and no known vision issue. Both of our representative participants were females and their ages are 27 and 24. All participants provided informed consent, and the study received approval from the Institutional Review Board (IRB) at the University of Alabama at Birmingham.
Performance on low and mid-level vision tasks with adaptive staircases
Figures 6 and 7 illustrate the performance progression of two participants across four specific visual tasks: visual acuity (Panel A), contrast sensitivity (Panel B), contour integration (Panel C), and crowding (Panel D). The performance trajectories are represented using color-coded staircases, where green denotes the left location of the target and purple signifies the right location. For each task, thresholds were calculated by averaging the last six reversals for each location (and for each shape or orientation in the contour integration and crowding tasks, respectively). These thresholds are marked by a dotted line perpendicular to the y-axis. It is important to note that for the visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, and crowding tasks, lower values on the y-axis correspond to better performance, whereas for the contour integration task, higher values indicate superior performance.
Figure 6. Performance of Participant 1 across tasks utilizing adaptive staircases: Panels A, B, C, and D correspond to the participant's performance on the visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, contour integration, and crowding tasks, respectively. Performance on the left location is marked by red dots, while performance on the right location is denoted by black dots. The thresholds for each task are represented by dashed lines perpendicular to the y-axis. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 7. Performance of Participant 2 across tasks utilizing adaptive staircases: Panels A, B, C, and D correspond to the participant's performance on the visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, contour integration, and crowding tasks, respectively. Performance on the left location is marked by red dots, while performance on the right location is denoted by black dots. The thresholds for each task are represented by dashed lines perpendicular to the y-axis. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Participants showed reliable performance in visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, contour integration and crowding tasks during the experiment (Figure 6 & 7).
Measures of Attention
Figure 8 illustrates the participants' performance on the exogenous attention task, where reaction times are measured for congruent (valid cue) and incongruent (invalid cue) trials, categorized by location (left/right). For Participant 1, a significant effect of cue type was observed at the left location (Welch’s t-test: t(111.5) = -2.6, p < 0.05), indicating a notable difference in reaction times based on cue congruency (Figure 8). However, no significant effect was found at the right location. For Participant 2, there was no significant effect of cueing observed at either location. Figure 8 shows consistent cueing effect, as expected in an exogenous attention task.
Figure 8. Exogenous Attention Task Analysis: The figure presents the reaction times (measured in seconds) of two participants, with data grouped according to the location of target presentation. In this visualization, blue bars represent reaction times and percent accuracy for congruent and incongruent trials. Error bars are included to indicate the standard deviation for each condition. Upper graphs show reaction times and accuracy rate on the left side, lower graphs show reaction times and accuracy rate on the right side. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Performance on Free Eye Movement Tasks
Performance on the MNRead task is measured by the time taken to read each sentence, with the task concluding when the participant can no longer read the sentence. Figure 9A and B display the MNRead task performance for both participants. As anticipated, the time required to read each sentence increases as the font size decreases. From these results, we can estimate key metrics such as reading acuity (the smallest font size correctly read), maximum reading speed, and the critical print size (the smallest print size at which participants can read at their maximum speed). These metrics can be compared both within and between participants. Figure 9C illustrates performance on the Trail Making Task, with total completion time recorded for both Part A (connecting numbers in ascending order) and Part B (connecting alternating numbers and letters in sequential order). Despite having the same number of elements, participants generally take longer to complete Part B, a finding consistent with previous research43.
Figure 9. Analyses of ecologically valid assessment tasks: The response time (measured in seconds) as a function of sentence font size is presented for Participant 1 in Panel A and for Participant 2 in Panel B. Panel C illustrates the time to completion (in seconds) for both Part A and Part B of the Trail Making Task. In this figure, blue bars represent the performance of Participant 1, while red bars correspond to the performance of Participant 2. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Eye Movement Analysis
To understand peripheral viewing strategies after visual training, we analyze fixation distributions to estimate fixation stability and the location of the PRL44,45. The dispersion of eye positions within a trial is analyzed by controlling for varying fixation locations across different trials. This approach allows for the calculation of the average dispersion of eye positions within each trial. This metric is a within-trial measure of the dispersion of eye positions after the first fixation of the trial, consistent with previous studies27,28. Moreover, perceptual training of healthy vision individuals by using gaze-contingent display leads to shorter saccade latency46. We analyze peripheral fixation behavior by calculating dispersion through determining the Bivariate Contour Ellipse Area (BCEA), which encompasses a specified percentage of fixations, typically 68%, over a certain time period (e.g., 15-30 seconds). Unlike previous studies, we normalized the dispersion of fixations for each trial-by-trial duration and then averaged this across all trials (as shown in Figure 10, column 2). This normalization ensures that even if fixations are centered in different locations across trials, the method plots all distributions at a common reference point. Additionally, we employed a probability density analysis using Kernel Density Estimation (KDE) to visually represent areas with a high density of fixations (Figure 10, column 3). This technique allows us to define PRL as the region corresponding to the peak of the KDE function. It is important to note that these analyses offer a general overview of participants' gaze patterns over time but do not distinguish between how gaze patterns vary from trial to trial.
Figure 10. Fixation Stability Analysis: The figure displays BCEA and KDE plots of fixation distributions for the two participants. In the BCEA plots, a blue ellipse encloses 68% of the total fixations. In the KDE plots, the bright yellow area indicates the region with the highest fixation density. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Supplemental File. Please click here to download this File.
In this methodological paper, we present a gaze-contingent framework for conducting perceptual research in simulated central vision loss that emphasizes hardware, design, and methodological considerations that are required to (1) choose the shortest system latency for gaze-contingent display, (2) administer a wide range of visual perception tasks, and (3) measure the oculomotor and perceptual performance of participants within this paradigm. Concerning (1), we emphasize the need for testing hardware and software reliability in maintaining the perception of gaze contingency. Specific combinations of eye tracker equipment and computer software and hardware, ideally more than one, should be tested to ensure that the latency is low enough not to disrupt the perception of contingency and, importantly, not to allow participants to exploit the system delay to perform tasks with their fovea. Concerning (2), we implemented several checks when designing tasks (and subsequently collecting data) using gaze-contingent displays. A key aspect is ensuring that participants become familiar with the modified viewing conditions introduced by the scotoma. This involves training them to maintain stable fixations, which is essential for fixation-dependent tasks, and initiating appropriate oculomotor behavior, both of which are crucial for successfully completing perceptual tasks. We achieve this by training participants to fixate with the scotoma and using a PRL induction task prior to use of the perceptual tasks. Concerning (3), we have a detailed framework for each task including set-up, instructions, practice, and adaptive methods addressing both free-viewing and fixation-controlled tasks. A distinctive feature of our framework is its capacity to accommodate a broad spectrum of perceptual tasks, designed to assess performance at various levels of visual processing, including low-, mid-, and high-level tasks. To accurately measure performance, it is essential to incorporate adequate breaks both within and between tasks, and to structure the psychophysical demands so that performance can be assessed efficiently and reliably. This approach minimizes fatigue, particularly in participants with intact central vision, who may otherwise experience strain from extended use of their visual periphery. Finally, we introduce methods to analyze eye-movements within-trials by calculating the BCEA encompassing a given percentage of fixations over a certain period to quantify oculomotor behavior representative of different peripheral-looking strategies of participants.
Using simulated central vision loss as a model to test the specificity and generalization of perceptual learning
The rationale for using simulated central vision loss is twofold: (1) it provides a controlled environment to test the training strategies and assess how learning transfers to other untrained tasks and locations, and (2) effective use of peripheral vision requires enhancement across multiple levels of visual processing, including low-, mid-, and high-level vision. Our goal is to investigate how these different visual domains evolve together through perceptual learning following central vision loss. By measuring a range of learning outcomes, we aim to characterize distinct learning profiles and patterns of generalization across various outcome measures.
Although not exhaustive, the proposed training strategies encompass all three domains of visual processing, addressing fundamental aspects of vision that are known to be, at least partially, separable from both visual performance and neuroscientific perspectives. In this study, participants undergo 20 training sessions, each lasting approximately 45 minutes, on one of four training tasks assigned randomly. Before training begins, each participant's specific preferred retinal locus (PRL) is identified through the PRL induction task, and this locus is then used as the trained retinal locus (TRL) during the training. Baseline and post-training performance on various assessment tasks are measured to observe whether learning transfers to other tasks and untrained locations (i.e., locations other than the TRL). Additionally, we examine pre- and post-training changes in eye movement metrics across these tasks.
Limitations
While this framework is currently utilized to train and assess performance in both individuals with healthy vision (using simulated scotomas) and patients with macular degeneration (MD), there are several limitations that warrant consideration. In our study, we employ a visible scotoma, which may lead to compensatory eye movements or other strategies that might not occur with a real-world, invisible scotoma. Additionally, the use of a static scotoma, as opposed to dynamic scotomas that can change and grow in shape and size (as seen in patients), limits our ability to study the longitudinal effects of central vision loss. However, it is feasible to monitor the physical properties of a scotoma in patients, such as size and shape, using microperimetry. Further, it is possible that the uniform background of scotoma leads to adaptation effects, and future research should examine use of non-uniform backgrounds. Furthermore, while we simulate central vision loss using computer displays, it is also crucial to examine its effects in more naturalistic settings. Extended reality (XR) systems offers the potential to provide a more immersive and subjective experience of simulated central vision loss for individuals with healthy vision, but it is essential to carefully consider the latency of such systems to ensure a smooth and realistic perception of the scotoma. Importantly the use of XR could also facilitate use of more naturalistic tasks that could better mimic real-world tasks that people with central vision loss need to navigate.
Conclusion
The proposed framework for using gaze-contingent displays to test vision in the context of simulated central vision loss has broad applicability both for understanding use of peripheral vision after central vision loss and for developing new vision training interventions. The novel multidimensional framework integrates multiple approaches to testing vision both in fixation controlled and free viewing conditions and can support gaze-contingent training interventions. Further aspects of the framework can be extended to address other conditions of retinal or cortically based visual field loss that also rely upon measurement and/or training that targets specific visual field locations in context of vision loss. It can also be translated across technology systems, such as modern extended reality systems, to provide greater accessibility for research and practice addressing low vision.
The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest regarding the publication of this paper.
This work is supported by NIH NEI 1 U01 R01EY031589 and 1R21EY033623-01.
Name | Company | Catalog Number | Comments |
CRT Monitor | ViewSonic PF817 Professional Series CRT, ViewSonic Corp. | https://www.viewsonic.com/us/monitors.html?srsltid= AfmBOorEmjc67A5U2v2V wywNRHWzdrxcYx7Q3Y0 9tiNrnbs6FC4TPlc9 | |
Display++ LCD Monitor | Cambridge Research Systems | https://www.crsltd.com/tools-for-vision-science/calibrated-displays/displaypp-lcd-monitor/ | |
Eye Tracker | EyeLink 1000 Plus Tower Mount, SR Research | https://www.sr-research.com/eyelink-1000-plus/ | |
Eye Tracker | Vpixx Technologies Inc. | www.vpixx.com | |
Macintosh IOS | Apple Inc. | https://www.apple.com/mac/ | |
Windows 10 | Microsoft Inc. | https://www.microsoft.com/en-us/ |
Request permission to reuse the text or figures of this JoVE article
Request PermissionThis article has been published
Video Coming Soon
Copyright © 2025 MyJoVE Corporation. All rights reserved